What Is the Reality of Achieving Speed Reading?

A commonly cited statistic from Dr. Keith Rayner, a professor at the University of Massachusetts Amherst, asserts that 95% of college-educated individuals read at a rate of 200–400 words per minute. Despite this, a vocal minority claims the ability to read much faster, several times this rate, using various speed reading techniques. Numerous companies and apps, too, promise dramatic increases in reading speed, some even suggesting a capability of 1,000 words per minute. A notable figure in this domain is Tim Ferriss, known for “The Four Hour Work Week,” who claims his method can increase reading speed by an average of 386% with just three hours of practice. But how much truth is there to these claims?

At the heart of the speed reading debate is the issue of comprehension. It’s of little use to read twice as fast if the understanding of the text suffers significantly. Numerous studies have shown that speed readers tend to score lower on comprehension tests compared to those reading at a normal pace. The fundamental question arises: Is it possible to maintain high comprehension while reading significantly faster?

Dr. Ronald Carver, author of “The Causes of High and Low Reading Achievement,” conducted a seminal study to address this question. He meticulously selected the best speed readers, including college students, professionals with high daily reading requirements, and individuals known for their speed reading abilities. The aim was to assess if these top performers could maintain high comprehension levels at speeds exceeding the average.

The results were telling. None of the elite readers in Carver’s study could maintain a 75% comprehension level (a C average) while reading above 600 words per minute. Though faster than an average college-educated adult, this speed still only represented a 1/3 increase over the upper limit of average reading speed.

Professor Keith Raynor, an authority in reading research, underscores a crucial point: very few people can read faster than 400 words per minute without a significant loss in comprehension. Raynor elaborates on the physiological limitations, noting the role of the fovea in the eye and how it impacts reading speed. The fovea’s sharp, central vision is essential for reading, and its acuity drops significantly outside this area. The speed at which the brain processes information also plays a role in limiting reading speed.

Debunking Common Speed Reading Techniques

The Limitations of Eye Movements and Saccades

Many speed reading methods focus on reducing eye movements or saccades, typically taking about a tenth of a second each. Techniques like reading two lines at once or absorbing several words before an eye movement are commonly promoted. However, Raynor points out the lack of evidence that one can take in more information per eye fixation or process two lines of text simultaneously.

The Role and Risks of Subvocalization

Another widespread speed reading technique involves reducing or eliminating subvocalization – the internal speech while reading. While there can be some benefits to minimizing this, research shows that comprehension suffers, especially with complex texts. Subvocalization plays a role in understanding and retaining the meaning of words; removing this pause can lead to looking at many words without truly grasping or remembering them.

Facts You Didn’t Know About Speed Reading

  • Speed reading techniques have been around for much longer than most people realize. While the concept gained popularity in the mid-20th century, its roots can be traced back to the early 1900s. One of the earliest known proponents of speed reading was Evelyn Wood, who developed her method in the 1950s.
  • There are several different methods of speed reading, and not all are equally effective. Some popular techniques include chunking (reading groups of words at once), minimizing subvocalization (inner speech), and using a pointer or guide (like a finger or pen) to maintain focus and pace.
  • The effectiveness of speed reading varies significantly depending on the type of material being read. It tends to be more effective for reading simple or familiar texts than for complex or dense materials, such as academic papers or technical manuals.
  • One of the keys to speed reading is reducing the number of eye fixations per line. The average reader has about 8 to 10 fixations per line, while a speed reader aims to reduce this to 3 to 4. However, this requires training the eyes and brain to take in more information at a glance.
  • Some speed reading methods attempt to train peripheral vision to increase word recognition at the edges of the reading line. This is based on expanding the ‘span of recognition’, although there’s limited scientific evidence to support its effectiveness.
  • Neuroplasticity, or the brain’s ability to reorganize itself, plays a role in speed reading. With practice, the brain can adapt to process information more rapidly, although there’s a natural cap to this speed dictated by cognitive processing abilities.
  • Speed reading techniques aren’t suitable for all readers. Individuals with reading disabilities or difficulties, such as dyslexia, often find these techniques challenging and not particularly helpful.
  • Skimming, which involves glancing over text to grasp the main ideas without going into detail, is often confused with speed reading. While both involve fast reading, speed reading ideally includes a more thorough understanding of the text.
  • The rise of digital media has altered the way we read, potentially impacting the effectiveness of traditional speed reading techniques. Screen reading often involves more scanning and non-linear navigation compared to print reading.
  • There is a surprising lack of long-term, comprehensive studies on the effectiveness of speed reading. Most research focuses on short-term results, and there is limited data on how speed reading skills are retained or evolve.

It becomes clear that while certain methods can increase reading speed, they often come with compromises in comprehension and retention. The physiological and cognitive limitations of the human brain and vision system place a natural cap on the extent to which reading speed can be enhanced. Techniques like reducing subvocalization or increasing peripheral vision span might offer some improvements, but they are not universally applicable and vary in effectiveness depending on the reader and the material.

The lack of long-term, comprehensive studies on the effectiveness and retention of speed reading skills further muddies the waters. Therefore, while some marginal gains in speed are achievable, they are typically not without their costs in terms of understanding and long-term learning.